psychiatrist

This work may not be copied, distributed, displayed, published, reproduced, transmitted, modified, posted, sold, licensed, or used for commercial purposes. By downloading this file, you are agreeing to the publisher’s Terms & Conditions.

Article

A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed for Comparing Processing Speed to Differentiate Adult Psychiatric Referrals With and Without Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders

Elisabeth Hemmersam Wiig, PhD, and Niels Peter Nielsen, MD

Published: March 29, 2012

 

A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed for Comparing Processing Speed to Differentiate Adult Psychiatric Referrals With and Without Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders

Vertical divider

ABSTRACT

Objective: This retrospective study used A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT) to compare processing speed and efficiency measures by adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) or non-ADHD psychiatric disorders and healthy controls.

Method: Color, form, and color-form combination naming tests were administered to 104 adults, ages 17-55 years, referred for psychiatric evaluation of possible ADHD. Thirty healthy adults were controls. Psychiatric intake procedures identified 64 adults with ADHD (ICD-10 and DSM-IV criteria) and 40 with mild psychiatric disorders without ADHD. The study was conducted from 2008 through 2010.

Results: At intake, color, form, and color-form combination naming times (seconds) were longer and overhead [color-form combination – (color + form)] was larger for patients with ADHD than for non-ADHD patients and controls. In the ADHD group, color and form measures were in the normal range. Color-form combination was in the slower-than-normal speed (60-70 seconds) and overhead, a processing-efficiency measure, in the atypical range (> 10 seconds). In the non-ADHD patient and control groups, all AQT measures were in the normal range. Analysis of variance with post hoc analysis of log-normal values for color, form, and color-form combination and time for overhead indicated significant (Bonferroni P < .01) mean differences between the ADHD and other groups, but not between the non-ADHD and control groups. When using fail criteria for either color-form combination or overhead, the sensitivity for the ADHD group was 89%.

Conclusions: Results support AQT as a possible complement to psychiatric intake procedures to differentiate adults with ADHD from those with mild psychiatric disorders, and they suggest that a controlled prospective study might be productive.

Prim Care Companion CNS Disord 2012;14(2):doi:10.4088/PCC.11m01273

Submitted: August 11, 2011; accepted November 9, 2011.

Published online: March 29, 2012.

Corresponding author: Elisabeth Hemmersam Wiig, PhD, 2131 Reflection Bay Drive, Arlington, TX 76013 (ehwiig@krii.com).

Behavioral and quality of life ratings are measurement standards in psychiatric intake procedures and for evaluating pharmacologic treatment effects in adolescents/adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders (ADHDs).1-6 A diagnosis of ADHD is based on observations of impulsivity, inattention, and hyperactivity, and distinguishing between ADHD and mild psychiatric disorders without ADHD based on behavioral ratings can be difficult.7 This retrospective study explored whether A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT) might provide complementary, objective measures of cognitive control to differentiate adults with ADHD from those with milder, nonorganic forms of psychiatric disorders without ADHD.8,9 A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed color-form naming has proven useful in assessing executive dysfunction secondary to neurologic/neuropsychiatric disorders in older adults.10-14 A prospective study provided preliminary evidence that the AQT additive model for processing speed and efficiency differentiated adults with ADHD and healthy controls.15,16

The use of processing-speed tests in clinical studies of ADHD symptomatology in children and adults has increased in recent years.17-21 This study used AQT color, form, and color-form combination naming to assess processing speed and efficiency. Dual-dimension color-form combination naming by healthy adults activates bilateral temporal-parietal and subcortical brain regions, including the hippocampus.8,9,13,21 The temporal-parietal lobes bilaterally have been related to working memory and white matter atrophy has been related to cognitive processing deficits.22,23 Other evidence supports that these regions are associated with focusing and reorienting attention, keeping visual items active in working memory, set shifting, and cognitive control.24-26 These executive functions are now widely substantiated to be impaired in children and adults with ADHD and to affect work performance negatively.2-4,27-33

This study evaluated the clinical utility of AQT in differentiating psychiatric referrals for evaluation of possible ADHD as either true or false. Adults with ADHD were expected to exhibit significantly slower processing speed and reduced processing efficiency compared to healthy controls.15 Adults with mild, nonorganic psychiatric disorders without ADHD (non-ADHD) were hypothesized to exhibit similar processing speed and efficiency as healthy controls. We anticipated that the AQT additive model would result in similar levels of sensitivity and specificity as a means for differentiating adults with ADHD from healthy controls.15,16

METHOD

Participants

Intake protocols, covering a 3-year period from 2008 through 2010, for 127 adults who were referred to a regional, urban outpatient clinic for psychiatric evaluation and who completed the AQT provided the database. All psychiatric diagnoses were determined primarily according to World Health Organization (WHO) ICD-10 criteria, as required by state regulations, and secondarily to DSM-IV-TR (314.01) criteria for validation.34,35 Of the 64 referrals, 28 female participants and 36 male participants (ages 17 to 54 years; mean = 31.14) received ADHD diagnoses (WHO ICD-10 code F90.0 or F90.9) and follow-up pharmacologic treatment. Five adults with ADHD diagnoses who did not complete treatment within the setting were excluded. Participants in the ADHD group exhibited impaired academic achievement, difficulties with employment, and comorbidities such as substance abuse and mild personality disorders or depression, all of which are commonly found in adults with ADHD.3,7,36-41 Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes, as delineated by DSM-IV-TR, were not further explored due to time constraints imposed by the social-medicine environment.

Forty referrals, 12 female and 28 male patients (ages 18 to 44 years; mean = 32.3), received diagnoses that included personality disorders (ICD-10 codes F60.0, F60.1, F60.31, F60.5, F60.6, F60.7, or F60.9), addiction (F10.1, F12.2, F19.1, F19.2, or F19.9), affective disorders (F32.9, F33.8, or F38.1), or obsessive-compulsive disorder (F42.9), but excluded ADHD. Some participants in this group experienced impaired academic performance; specific learning disabilities, such as dyslexia; and/or difficulties with employment. Eighteen psychiatric referrals with autism spectrum syndrome (F84.0, F84.1), organic brain disorders (F04.9, F06.2, and F07.0), or bipolar disorder/schizophrenia (F20.0, F20.6, F20.9, F21.9, F23.1, F31.9, and F45.1) were excluded from the study. All participants with psychiatric disorders consented to be assessed with AQT at intake before medication or other treatment.

Thirty healthy adults, 14 female and 16 male, ranging in age from 18 to 43 years (mean = 28.27), who participated in an earlier study, were controls.15 Controls were recruited from the urban community in response to announcements to personal contacts. Single-factor analysis of variance (ANOVA) indicated that participants in the 3 groups did not differ significantly in age (F1,131 = 2.29, P = .11).

clinical points
  • Measures of processing speed in A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed may complement standard psychiatric intake procedures to differentiate adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder from referrals with mild psychiatric disorders.
  • Current evidence indicates a high degree of clinical utility (sensitivity 89%) when applying fail criteria for dual-dimension processing of color-form combinations and overhead, a measure of processing efficiency.
  • A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed may provide objective, clinical baselines for evaluating the effects of treatment with pharmaceuticals.

Materials and Administration

The color, form, and color-form combination naming tests of A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed were administered individually, in standard order (ie, color, form, and then color-form combination) to all psychiatric referrals at intake without medication and to all controls. The single-dimension color task requires rapid naming of 40 randomly sequenced, colored squares (black, blue, red, yellow), and the form task requires naming of black shapes (circles, lines, squares, triangles). The color and form tests measure reaction, retrieval, and response time. The dual-dimension color-form combination test requires rapid naming of combinations of the colors and shapes (eg, red circle). In healthy adults, the color-form combination test measures an additional “switch cost” derived from articulating 2 words in rapid sequence.8,9,16 In neuropsychiatric disorders, there is an additional penalty that is caused by increased demands on attentional control, visual working memory, and cognitive set shifting.10-13 The naming times for each test (seconds) and a measure of the difference (overhead) between color-form combination naming and the sum of color and form naming times [color-form combination – (color + form)] provided the data. Overhead measures the relative efficiency of dual-dimension processing, including switch cost, and it was norm-referenced with 270 healthy adults, ages 18 to 65 years.16 A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed color, form, and color-form combination tests are highly reliable (r = 0.91-0.95), and there is no evidence of learning or habituation during repeated (10 minute) naming.8,9 In healthy development, maximum color-form combination processing speed is reached at age 15 years, with minimal change with advancing age (1 s/decade).42,43

The same psychiatrist administered AQT concurrently with psychiatric interviews and completion of Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS-v1.1)44 during a standard psychiatric work-up that concurred with prevailing Danish and European psychiatric practice and started with a referral for a diagnostic evaluation of possible ADHD symptomatology.45 Intake procedures consisted of (1) a structured psychiatric interview, (2) administration of ASRS-v1.1, (3) administration of AQT for baseline purposes, (4) and acceptance of possible treatment with informed consent. At intake, participants with ADHD diagnosis (n = 64) received ASRS-v1.1 ratings, based on all items, ranging from 22 to 76 (mean = 50.69, SD = 11.56) and indicating probable ADHD (ie, > 20 points). Participants with non-ADHD diagnoses (eg, personality disorders, addiction, mild depression) received ASRS-v1.1 ratings ranging from 22 to 68 (mean = 49.41, SD = 11.85). Comparison of group mean values indicated no significant difference in the ASRS-v1.1 ratings in the 2 psychiatric groups (t = 0.47, P = .64).

Statistical Analyses

All analyses of data were performed with the Microsoft Excel StatPlus, Mac 2009 version (Microsoft, Redmond, Washington). One-way ANOVA with post hoc analysis (Bonferroni) compared mean differences in processing speed, as measured by color, form, and color-form combination, between the 3 groups. Paired-sample t tests, not assuming equal variance, compared group mean values for the overhead [color-form combination – (color + form)]. Null hypotheses were rejected at P < .01.

RESULTS

Descriptive statistics for the AQT naming times and overhead measures (seconds) for the 3 groups (ADHD, non-ADHD, and controls) are shown in Table 1. At intake, color and form naming-time mean values were within the normal ranges for all groups (ie, < 25 and < 30 seconds, respectively). The ADHD group mean for color-form combination naming was in the upper range of slower-than-normal speed (ie, between 60-70 seconds) and, for overhead, in the atypical/pathological range (ie, > 10 seconds). In the non-ADHD and control groups, the mean values for color-form combination and overhead were within the normal range (ie, < 60 seconds and < 6 seconds, respectively).8,9,16 Figure 1 illustrates the naming-time differences (seconds) between groups for each measure.

Table 1

Click figure to enlarge

Figure 1

Click figure to enlarge

Tests for normality of the sampling distributions for each AQT processing-speed variable accepted this assumption for all measures. Assumptions for homogeneity of variance were rejected, and we applied log-normal transformations to all color, form, and color-form combination naming times. One-way ANOVA with post hoc analysis (Bonferroni) evaluated the significance of mean differences in the AQT log-normal measures between groups (Table 2). The mean values for color (F1,131 = 13.46, P = .0000, η2 = 0.17), form (F1,131 = 17.78, P < .0000, η2 = 0.21), and color-form combination naming (F1,131 = 77.91, P < .0000, η2 = 0.54) differed significantly between groups. Effect sizes ranged from small for color and form to large for color-form combination naming.

Table 2

Click figure to enlarge

Overhead times were not transformed to log-normal values because they could be either positive or negative. The significance of mean differences was tested with 2-tailed t tests, not assuming equivalence of variance. Overhead mean values differed significantly between the ADHD and non-ADHD groups (t1,103 = 9.29; P = .0000; Cohen d = 1.74, r = 0.63) and between the ADHD and control groups (t1,93 = 8.40; P < .0000; Cohen d = 1.60, r = 0.63), and effect sizes were large. There was no significant mean difference between the non-ADHD psychiatric and control groups (t1,69 = 1.32; P > .05; Cohen d = −0.31, r = −0.16). On the basis of the ADHD overhead mean, the mean switch cost plus penalty overhead for 40 color-form combination items was 330 ms/stimulus, indicating a sizeable effect of demands on attention, visual working memory, and set shifting. In comparison, the mean switch cost was 80 ms/stimulus in the control group and 50 ms/stimulus in the non-ADHD group.

As results were analyzed, the question emerged whether the intake measures would be consistent if AQT were administered twice before the initiation of pharmacologic treatment. On examination, 45 adults in the ADHD group and 27 in the non-ADHD group were given the AQT tests with short time intervals ranging from a day to a week. Mean values for each AQT variable for these administrations were compared with simple 1-way ANOVA, as the paired sample distributions met assumptions for normality and homogeneity of variance. In the ADHD group, paired mean values for first and second administrations did not differ significantly (P > .05) for color (25.22 and 25.51 seconds, F1,88 = 0.0531), form (29.87 and 29.31 seconds, F1,88 = 0.1424), color-form combination (65.96 and 64.29 seconds, F1,88 = 0.4330), or overhead (11.47 and 10.07 seconds, F1,88 = 0.4024). In the non-ADHD group, paired mean values for first and second administrations also did not differ significantly (P > .05) for color (20.46 and 21.78 seconds, F1,52 = 0.7379), form (24.48 and 24.52 seconds, F1,52 = 0.0011), color-form combination (47.11 and 48.63 seconds, F1,52 = 0.7816), and overhead (1.48 and 2.37 seconds, F1,52 = 0.5075).

Sensitivity and specificity were determined by comparing individual naming times to criterion-referenced cut-off times at +1 SD of the mean for the upper limits of healthy performance.9,10,16 Table 3 shows the pass/fail percentages for each AQT measure in the 3 groups. The sensitivity in identifying adults with ADHD (n = 64) at intake was 80% for the overhead and 89% when fail criteria for either color-form combination naming or overhead were applied in combination. Specificity proved highest for the AQT color-form combination processing-speed measure (100%).

Table 3

Click figure to enlarge

We used linear least squares regression analyses to establish how well the sum of the color + form naming times would predict color-form combination naming for each population. The resulting coefficients of determination (R2) were 0.980 for the ADHD group, 0.986 for the non-ADHD group, and 0.951 for the control group. Figure 2 indicates extensive separation of the regression lines between the ADHD and the non-ADHD and control groups, which are similar. The differences in the slopes indicate that, in the ADHD group, the increase in switch cost is proportionally larger as these participants obtain slower processing-speed measures than those in the non-ADHD and control groups. These results indicate that the sum of the single-dimension naming times (color + form) may have strong predictive capability of whether an individual has ADHD or is a psychiatric referral without ADHD or is healthy.

Figure 2

Click figure to enlarge

DISCUSSION

Some of the limitations of this study were imposed by time restrictions set by the social-medicine system within which it was conducted. The greatest limitation appears to be that subtypes of ADHD and existing comorbidities were not explored in greater detail. However, all participants with ADHD completed pharmacologic treatment with either methylphenidate or atomoxetine, with positive outcomes in that dual-dimension processing speed and efficiency (overhead) were restored to normal levels at stabilization of the ADHD symptomatology. The heterogeneity among participants with mild psychiatric disorders and associated difficulties presents a second limitation. It should be noted that only 2 participants in this group exhibited specific learning disabilities, such as dyslexia. The results should be considered from a clinical perspective and viewed as preliminary until independently validated. Notwithstanding, the results supported some of the stated hypotheses while negating others. Moreover, the present findings concur with a previous study of adults with ADHD and age- and sex-matched healthy controls.15 The outcomes of this and the previous study will therefore be compared throughout the discussion. We will also discuss the outcome of the comparison of ASRS-v1.1 ratings in the 2 psychiatric groups, although we did not form a priori hypotheses about these findings. Rapid naming deficits can occur with psychiatric and psychoeducational disorders, and it is appropriate to compare AQT color-form with the Stroop and Rapid Automatized Naming and Rapid Alternating Stimulus Test (RAN/RAS) naming tests.46,47

The design of AQT is similar to the Stroop Color-Word Test, as both assess single-dimension naming in 2 tasks and combine the 2 dimensions for dual-dimension naming. The executive function and the associated brain regions involved in performing the dual-dimension tasks on the 2 tests differ, however. The Stroop assesses interference inhibition deficits that reflect fronto-striatal underactivation associated with, for example, ADHD,48,49 whereas AQT color-form combination is associated with bilateral temporal-parietal activation.8,9,13,21 The RAN component of RAN/RAS requires sequential single-dimension naming, and total pause time is a strong predictor of early reading accuracy and fluency.50 The RAS component requires rapid naming of sequentially arranged alternating stimuli (letters, numbers, and colors). The RAN/RAS does not feature stimuli that combine previously tested single-dimension naming, and heavy emphasis is placed on naming symbolic stimuli that appear to be critical in differentiating dyslexia and ADHD.19

On the basis of ASRS-v1.1 ratings, all participants with ADHD obtained ratings above the cut-off criterion for normalcy, and the average was well within the range considered to indicate possible ADHD. Moreover, the ASRS-v1.1 ratings were not significantly different for adults in the ADHD or non-ADHD psychiatric groups. This finding indicates that no clear differentiation between ADHD and mild psychiatric disorders without ADHD could be made on the basis of the ASRS-v1.1 self-ratings.

The outcomes of group comparisons of the AQT processing speed and efficiency measures generally supported a priori predictions. It was, however, unexpected that participants with ADHD were significantly slower at processing and naming the single-dimension stimuli, even though the group mean values were in the normal range. It was anticipated that adults with ADHD would be slower and less efficient in processing dual-dimension stimuli than healthy controls. These findings concurred with our hypotheses and with the earlier comparison study of adults with ADHD and healthy controls.15 We tested the consistency of processing speed and efficiency measures, obtained at intake with 2 administrations of AQT, and found that group mean values were consistent over time in the ADHD and non-ADHD psychiatric groups. It is notable that, for adults with ADHD at intake without medication, the mean dual-dimension naming speed was in the slower-than-normal range (ie, 60-70 seconds) and the average overhead in the atypical/pathological range (> 6 seconds) compared to criteria for normalcy.8,9,16 These findings indicate that cognitive control of attention, working memory, and set shifting were significantly reduced by the ADHD symptomatology. On average, cognitive control was reduced by a factor of 4 compared to normative controls as a result of the increased demands on executive functions by color-form combination naming, and this agrees with previous findings.15 The sensitivity with which AQT dual-dimension processing speed (color-form combination) and efficiency (overhead) in combination identified adults with ADHD was somewhat lower (89%) in this than in the previous study (93%), but it was still in the high range.15

It was unexpected that the non-ADHD group performed within the normal range on all AQT measures. On the basis of ASRS-v1.1 self-ratings, it would be assumed that there would be some degree of reduction of cognitive control. This appeared not to be the case, as there were no significant differences in processing speed or efficiency in the non-ADHD compared to the control group. The average switch cost imposed by dual-dimension color-form combination naming in the non-ADHD group was relatively short at 50 ms/stimulus and compared well with the control group and normative data.15,16 The percentages that passed dual-dimension color-form combination naming criteria for normalcy in the non-ADHD and control groups were similar and high.

Our findings suggest that AQT dual-dimension naming speed and overhead measures can differentiate psychiatric referrals with ASRS-v1.1 ratings that indicate possible ADHD and differentiate true- from false-positive results. The illustration of results from linear least-squares regression analyses (see Figure 2) indicates that the switch cost imposed by ADHD symptomatology clearly separates adults in the ADHD group from those in the non-ADHD and control groups and thus supports the AQT additive model. Our findings should be validated in a prospective study with control of psychiatric and psychoeducational comorbidities, as these may have additive effects on the presenting ADHD symptomatology and executive dysfunctions.36-41 Independent validation could have vast clinical implications for daily psychiatric practice, costs, and integration of adults with ADHD in the work force of a complex society.27 Adult psychiatric referrals identified to fit the AQT additive-model profile for ADHD could be immediately placed on pharmacologic treatment trial for responsiveness.15,16 Adult referrals without organic brain disorders or pervasive developmental disorders, identified to fit the normal AQT performance profile (non-ADHD), could be referred for cognitive-behavioral therapy or other forms of rehabilitation. Within the limitations of this study, these preliminary findings suggest that validation studies with greater imposed control of variables might be productive.

Drug names: atomoxetine (Strattera), methylphenidate (Daytrana, Ritalin, and others).

Author affiliations: Communication Disorders, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts (Dr Wiig), and Department of Psychiatry, Hvidovre Hospital, Copenhagen, Denmark (Dr Nielsen).

Potential conflicts of interest: Drs Wiig and Nielsen receive royalty shares for A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed from Pearson Assessment.

Funding/support: None reported.

Acknowledgment: The authors gratefully acknowledge the support given by the study participants, who consented to be evaluated with a nonstandard test procedure.

REFERENCES

1. Adler LA, Spencer T, Brown TE, et al. Once-daily atomoxetine for adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a 6-month, double-blind trial. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2009;29(1):44-50. PubMed doi:10.1097/JCP.0b013e318192e4a0

2. Barkley RA. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorders: A Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Guilford; 2006.

3. Barkley R, Murphy KR, Fischer M. ADHD in Adults: What the Science Says. New York, NY: Guilford; 2008.

4. Brown TE, Holdnack J, Saylor K, et al. Effect of atomoxetine on executive function impairments in adults with ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2011;15(2):130-138. PubMed doi:10.1177/1087054709356165

5. Dittmann RW, Wehmeier PM, Schacht A, et al. Atomoxetine treatment and ADHD-related difficulties as assessed by adolescent patients, their parents and physicians. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2009;3(1):21. PubMed doi:10.1186/1753-2000-3-21

6. World Health Organization. Adult Self-Report Scale (ASRS-V1.1). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2003.

7 Kieling R, Rohde LA. ADHD in children and adults: diagnosis and prognosis. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2012;9:1-16. PubMed

8. Wiig EH, Nielsen NP, Minthon L, et al. A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT). San Antonio, TX: Pearson/PsychCorp; 2002.

9. Wiig EH, Nielsen NP, Minthon L, et al. AQT: Assessment of Parietal Function. Svensk Version & Norsk Versjon. Stockholm, Sweden: Pearson/PsychCorp; 2003.

10. Andersson M, Wiig EH, Minthon L, et al. A Quick Test for Cognitive Speed: a measure of cognitive speed in dementia with Lewy bodies. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2007;22(4):313-318. PubMed doi:10.1177/1533317507303198

11. Nielsen NP, Wiig EH, Warkentin S, et al. Clinical utility of color-form naming in Alzheimer’s disease: preliminary evidence. Percept Mot Skills. 2004;99(3, pt 2):1201-1204. PubMed

12. Palmqvist S, Minthon L, Wattmo C, et al. A Quick test of cognitive speed is sensitive in detecting early treatment response in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Res Ther. 2010;2(5):29. doi:10.1186/alzrt53 PubMed

13. Warkentin S, Erikson C, Janciauskiene S. rCBF pathology in Alzheimer’s disease is associated with slow processing speed. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(5):1193-1200. PubMed doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.08.029

14. Wiig EH, Annas P, Basun H, et al. The stability of AQT processing speed, ADAS-Cog and MMSE during acetylcholinesterase inhibitor treatment in Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurol Scand. 2010;121(3):186-193. PubMed doi:10.1111/j.1600-0404.2009.01160.x

15. Nielsen NP, Wiig EH. AQT cognitive speed and processing efficiency differentiate adults with and without ADHD: a preliminary study. Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract. 2011;15(3):219-227. doi:10.3109/13651501.2011.582538 PubMed

16. Nielsen NP, Wiig EH. An additive model for relations between AQT single- and dual-dimension naming speed. Percept Mot Skills. 2011;112(2):499-508. PubMed doi:10.2466/22.27.PMS.112.2.499-508

17. Bedard AC, Ickowicz A, Tannock R. Methylphenidate improves Stroop naming speed, but not response interference, in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2002;12(4):301-309. PubMed doi:10.1089/104454602762599844

18. Jacobson LA, Ryan M, Martin RB, et al. Working memory influences processing speed and reading fluency in ADHD. Child Neuropsychol. 2011;17(3):209-224. PubMed doi:10.1080/09297049.2010.532204

19. Katz LJ, Brown FC, Roth RM, et al. Processing speed and working memory performance in those with both ADHD and a reading disorder compared with those with ADHD alone. Arch Clin Neuropsych. 2011;26(5):425-433. PubMed

20. Larochette AC, Harrison AG, Rosenblum Y, et al. Additive neurocognitive deficits in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and depressive symptoms. Arch Clin Neuropsychol.2011;26(5):385-395. PubMed

21. Wiig EH, Nielsen NP, Minthon L, et al. A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT): efficacy of a new paradigm for cognitive screening [poster]. Presented at the Alzheimer’s Association International Conference on Alzheimer’s Disease; July 26-31, 2008; Chicago, IL.

22. Berryhill ME, Chein J, Olson IR. At the intersection of attention and memory: the mechanistic role of the posterior parietal lobe in working memory. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(5):1306-1315. PubMed doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.033

23. Turken A, Gabriel SW, Bammer R, et al. Cognitive processing speed and the structure of white matter pathways: convergent evidence from normal variations and lesion sites. Neuroimage. 2008;42(2):1023-1044. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.057

24. Esterman M, Chiu YC, Tamber-Rosenau BJ, et al. Decoding cognitive control in human parietal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(42):17974-17979. PubMed doi:10.1073/pnas.0903593106

25. Helenius P, Laasonen M, Hokkanen L, et al. Impaired engagement of the ventral attentional pathway in ADHD. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(7):1889-1896. PubMed doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.014

26. Makris N, Buka SL, Biederman J, et al. Attention and executive systems abnormalities in adults with childhood ADHD: A DT-MRI study of connections. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(5):1210-1220. PubMed doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm156

27. de Graaf R, Kessler RC, Fayyad J, et al. The prevalence and effects of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on the performance of workers: results from the WHO World Mental Health Survey Initiative. Occup Environ Med. 2008;65(12):835-842. PubMed doi:10.1136/oem.2007.038448

28. Biederman J, Petty CR, Fried R, et al. Stability of executive function deficits into young adult years: a prospective longitudinal follow-up study of grown up males with ADHD. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2007;116(2):129-136. PubMed doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01008.x

29. Hervey AS, Epstein JN, Curry JF. Neuropsychology of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(3):485-503. PubMed doi:10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.485

30. Kessler RC, Adler LA, Barkley R, et al. Patterns and predictors of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder persistence into adulthood: results from the National Comorbidity Survey replication. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1442-1451. PubMed doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.001

31. Martinussen R, Hayden J, Hogg-Johnson S, et al. A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44(4):377-384. PubMed doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000153228.72591.73

32 Rohlf H, Jucksch V, Gawrilow C, et al. Set shifting and working memory in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders. J Neural Transm. 2012;119(1):95-106. PubMed

33. Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, et al. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1336-1346. PubMed doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006

34. World Health Organization. ICD-10 Psykiske lidelser og adfærdsmæssige forstyrelser. Copenhagen, Denmark: Munksgaard; 2003.

35. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.

36. In de Braek D, Dijkstra JB, Jolles J. Cognitive complaints and neuropsychological functioning in adults with and without attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder referred for multidisciplinary assessment. Appl Neuropsychol. 2011;18(2):127-135. PubMed doi:10.1080/09084282.2011.570614

37. Montano CB, Weisler R. Distinguishing symptoms of ADHD from other psychiatric disorders in the adult primary care setting. Postgrad Med. 2011;123(3):88-98. PubMed

38. Faraone SV, Kunwar A, Adamson J, et al. Personality traits among ADHD adults: implications of late-onset and subthreshold diagnoses. Psychol Med. 2009;39(4):685-693. PubMed doi:10.1017/S0033291708003917

39. Nylander L, Holmqvist M, Gustafson L, et al. ADHD in adult psychiatry: minimum rates and clinical presentation in general psychiatry outpatients. Nord J Psychiatry. 2009;63(1):64-71. PubMed doi:10.1080/08039480802416323

40. Torgersen T, Gjervan B, Rasmussen K. ADHD in adults: a study of clinical characteristics, impairment and comorbidity. Nord J Psychiatry. 2006;60(1):38-43. PubMed doi:10.1080/08039480500520665

41. Wilens TE, Biederman J, Faraone SV, et al. Presenting ADHD symptoms, subtypes, and comorbid disorders in clinically referred adults with ADHD. J Clin Psychiatry. 2009;70(11):1557-1562. PubMed doi:10.4088/JCP.08m04785pur

42. Jacobson JM, Nielsen NP, Minthon L, et al. Multiple rapid automatic naming measures of cognition: normal performance and effects of aging. Percept Mot Skills. 2004;98(3, pt 1):739-753. PubMed doi:10.2466/pms.98.3.739-753

43. Wiig EH, Nielsen NP, Jacobson JM. A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed: patterns of age groups 15 to 95 years. Percept Mot Skills. 2007;104(3, pt 2):1067-1075. PubMed

44. Adler L, Kessler RC, Spencer T, eds. Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS-v1.1) Symptom Checklist. New York, NY: World Health Organization; 2004.

45. Kooij SJJ, Bejerot S, Blackwell A, et al. European consensus statement on diagnosis and treatment of adult ADHD: The European Network Adult ADHD. BMC Psychiatry. 2010;10(1):67. PubMed doi:10.1186/1471-244X-10-67

46. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18(6):643-662. doi:10.1037/h0054651

47. Wolf M, Denckla MB. The Rapid Automatized Naming and Rapid Alternating Stimulus Test RAN/RAS: Examiner’s Manual. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 2005.

48. Cubillo A, Halari R, Giampietro V, et al. Fronto-striatal underactivation during interference inhibition and attention allocation in grown up children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and persistent symptoms. Psychiatry Res. 2011;193(1):17-27. PubMed doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.12.014

49. Cubillo A., Halari R., Smith A, et al. Review of fronto-striatal and fronto-cortical brain abnormalities in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorders (ADHD) and new evidence for dysfunction in adults with ADHD during motivation and attention disorders [published online ahead of print April 27, 2011]. Cortex. PubMed

50. Georgiou GK, Parilla R, Kirby JR, et al. Rapid naming components and their relationship with phonological awareness, orthographic knowledge, speed of processing, and different reading outcomes. Sci Stud Read. 2008;12(4):325-350. doi:10.1080/10888430802378518

Related Articles

Volume: 14

Quick Links:

$40.00

Buy this Article as a PDF

References