<p class="frontmatter-fieldnotes disclaimernew" style="margin-bottom:15px;">This work may not be copied, distributed, displayed, published, reproduced, transmitted, modified, posted, sold, licensed, or used for commercial purposes. By downloading this file, you are agreeing to the publisher’s <a href="/pages/termsofuse.aspx" target="_blank">Terms & Conditions</a>.</p>
<div id="x12f01341">
<div class="story">
<img src="/Publishingimages/2011_rounds.gif"/>
<p class="title-left"><span class="bold">Found in Feces: Differential Diagnosis, Workup, and Treatment</span></p>
<p class="byline-left"><span class="italic">Theodore A. Stern, MD; J. Carl Pallais, MD, MPH; Jeremiah M. Scharf, MD, PhD; and Steven C. Schlozman, MD</span></p>
<div id="abstract"><img src="/Publishingimages/new_articles/grey_side_rule.jpg" alt="Vertical divider" />
<p class="subheads-abstract-head"><span class="semibold">LESSONS LEARNED AT THE INTERFACE OF MEDICINE AND PSYCHIATRY</span></p>
<p class="abstract-text">The Psychiatric Consultation Service at Massachusetts General Hospital sees medical and surgical inpatients with comorbid psychiatric symptoms and conditions. Such consultations require the integration of medical and psychiatric knowledge. During their twice-weekly rounds, Dr Stern and other members of the Consultation Service discuss the diagnosis and management of conditions confronted. These discussions have given rise to rounds reports that will prove useful for clinicians practicing at the interface of medicine and psychiatry.</p>
<p class="abstract-text"><span class="bold">Dr Stern</span> is chief of the Psychiatric Consultation Service at Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, and a professor of psychiatry at Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts. <span class="bold">Dr Pallais</span> is an assistant professor of medicine at Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, and an attending physician on the Clinician Educator Service and a subspecialty education coordinator in the Department of Endocrinology at Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston. <span class="bold">Dr Scharf </span>is director of Partners Neurology Tic Disorders Program at Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, and an assistant professor of psychiatry and neurology at Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts. <span class="bold">Dr Schlozman </span>is the associate director of Training, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry at Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, and McLean Hospital, Belmont, Massachusetts; is a staff child and general psychiatrist at Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston; and is codirector of medical student education in psychiatry and an assistant professor of psychiatry at Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts. </p>
<p class="abstract-text"><span class="bold">Dr Stern</span> is an employee of the Academy of Psychosomatic Medicine, has served on the speaker’s board of Reed Elsevier, and has received royalties from Mosby/Elsevier and McGraw Hill. <span class="bold">Drs Pallais</span>, <span class="bold">Scharf</span>, and <span class="bold">Schlozman</span> report no financial or other affiliations related to the subject of this article.</p>
<p class="abstract-citation"><span class="italic">Prim Care Companion CNS Disord 2012;14(3):</span><span class="doi italic">doi:10.4088/PCC.12f01341</span></p>
<p class="abstract-copyright"><span class="italic">© Copyright 2012 Physicians Postgraduate Press, Inc.</span></p>
<p class="front-fieldnotes first"><span class="italic">Submitted:</span> January 5, 2012; accepted March 27, 2012.</p>
<p class="front-fieldnotes"><span class="italic">Published online:</span> June 21, 2012.</p>
<p class="front-fieldnotes"><span class="italic">Funding/support: </span>None reported.</p>
<p class="front-fieldnotes"><span class="italic">Corresponding author:</span> Theodore A. Stern, MD, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Fruit St, WRN 605, Boston, MA 02114.</p>
</div>
<p class="drop-cap-with-body-text"><span class="bold">H</span>ave you ever seen a patient covered in feces and wondered why it happened? Can you imagine a “normal” person behaving in such a fashion? Have you felt disgusted and thought that your reaction might have affected the care you provided? If you have, then the following case vignette should serve as a stimulus for further discussion and clinical guidance and prepare you for the notion that certain situations direct medical care more than do symptoms.</p>
<p class="subheads-subhead-1"><span class="bold">CASE VIGNETTE</span></p>
<p class="body-text">Ms A, a 68-year-old woman, was found amid her feces and urine following a mechanical fall. When a neighbor came by to check on her, he called for an ambulance. The emergency medical technicians noted that the room temperature was 40°F (secondary to a furnace malfunction), yet she had not called for assistance. While Ms A had managed to obtain fluids, she spent several days on her couch. She described herself as an independent woman and stated that she wanted to be left alone. Her house was described as “a mess.” Preliminary examination by the emergency medical technicians revealed a probable hip fracture, and she was transported to the hospital for an open reduction and internal fixation. Ms A acknowledged that she was distraught over the death of her cat.</p>
<p class="body-text">Ms A’s past medical history was notable for a gait disturbance. Laboratory assessment included a serum urea nitrogen level of 4 mmol/L; a creatinine level of 0.46 μmol/L; a white blood cell count of 12,000/μL; and an albumin level of 4.6 g/L.</p>
<p class="body-text">Physical examination revealed an awake, alert, polite, cooperative, and fluent but dysarthric 80-lb woman. Her Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref1">1</a></span> score was 28/30. However, Ms A had problems with 3-step Luria maneuvers, was perseverative, and manifest poor planning on clock-drawing. In addition, she thought that the tallest building in Boston, Massachusetts, was 6 ft tall and that the average loaf of bread had 100 slices. Nonetheless, initial evaluations by clinical staff did not detect a psychiatric disturbance.</p>
<p class="subheads-subhead-1"><span class="bold">WHAT IS THE DIFFERENTIAL DIAGNOSIS OF BEING FOUND IN FECES?</span></p>
<p class="body-text">The differential diagnosis of being found in feces is broad. Although being covered in stool may be a normal occurrence in certain professions (eg, sewage engineering), under most circumstances, being covered in fecal waste is a marker of underlying pathology.</p>
<p class="body-text">Despite the fact that people produce solid waste from digested material on a regular basis, being covered in stool is relatively uncommon. The low frequency of this finding is due largely to our society’s investment in significant resources to ensure that bodily wastes are disposed of in a neat, hygienic manner. From an early age, toddlers learn that stool is an undesirable bodily product and contact with it should be minimized. The vast infrastructure devoted to ensuring adequate sanitation practices (and that decrease the public’s risk of infection) reinforces the lessons learned about stool management during potty training.</p>
<p class="body-text">A practical approach to thinking about an individual who is brought to a medical facility covered in fecal matter involves consideration of both abnormal stool excretion and impaired response to fecal contamination. From a social perspective, the more stool on one’s body, the greater the abnormality. Although many individuals have “accidents” in the setting of severe diarrhea, a rapid response time (for cleaning oneself) is expected to avoid social scorn. Thus, factors that impact the rate and quantity of stool production as well as the time to clean up the mess are considered when creating a differential diagnosis.</p>
<div class="clinical-article">
<div class="heading"><img src="/Publishingimages/new_tocs/clinical_points.gif" alt="clinical points" /></div>
<p class="take-home-head-clin-pts"></p>
<ul>
<li class="take-home-list">The differential diagnosis of being found in feces is broad.</li>
<li class="take-home-list">Those who let themselves be found in stool can be classified into 4 broad categories: those who are not aware that there is a problem, those who cannot move, those who will not move, and those who do not move.</li>
<li class="take-home-list">While the Mini-Mental State Examination can uncover moderate-to-severe impairments in cognitive function, it is insensitive to deficits in executive function; thus, more extensive mental status testing may be required during the assessment of neuropsychiatric dysfunction.</li>
</ul>
</div>
<p class="subheads-subhead-1"><span class="bold">WHAT FACTORS INFLUENCE STOOL PRODUCTION?</span></p>
<p class="body-text">Factors that influence stool production and fecal soiling need to be considered in the workup of a person brought in for medical attention after being found in feces.</p>
<p class="body-text">Diarrhea, with high quantities or fast flow of stool (and with attendant increases in the risk of fecal soiling), has myriad causes. Infections, medications, endocrine disorders, and several malabsorptive and inflammatory conditions can cause diarrhea and increase the likelihood that a patient may become covered in feces.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref2">2–5</a></span> Other conditions (eg, ostomy malfunction, short-bowel syndrome, irritable bowel syndrome, diabetic autonomic neuropathy) can also place an increased burden on a patient’s ability to remain free from fecal waste.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref3">3</a></span></p>
<p class="body-text">At the other end of the spectrum, constipation can cause fecal soiling due to liquid stool leakage around an impacted fecal mass, a condition referred to as overflow incontinence or encopresis.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref3">3</a>,<a href="#ref5">5</a>,<a href="#ref6">6</a></span> In general, constipation is multifactorial; however, it can be categorized by conditions that slow intestinal transit or by those that result in defecatory dysfunction.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref7">7</a>,<a href="#ref8">8</a></span> Slow transit constipation can be caused by myopathy or enteric neuropathy, systemic and metabolic conditions (eg, hypothyroidism, hypercalcemia, amyloidosis, spinal cord injury, multiple sclerosis), or various drugs that reduce intestinal motility (eg, opiates, anticholinergics).<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref8">8</a></span> Defecatory disorders can arise from impaired rectal sensitivity, abnormal sphincter function, spastic floor syndrome, or structural abnormalities (such as rectocele, rectal intussusception, or rectal cancer).<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref7">7</a>,<a href="#ref8">8</a> </span>Diet, hydration status, physical activity, and voluntary responses all influence the development of constipation. However, it is important to note that only a small subset of individuals with constipation experience overflow impaction, encopresis, or fecal soiling.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref6">6</a>,<a href="#ref8">8</a></span></p>
<p class="body-text">In addition to constipation and diarrhea, other risk factors for adult fecal incontinence include female gender, pregnancy, advanced age, poor health status, neurologic disorders, and institutionalized residence.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref5">5</a></span></p>
<p class="subheads-subhead-1"><span class="bold">WHAT TYPES OF RESPONSES TO FECAL SOILING ARE ABNORMAL?</span></p>
<p class="body-text">One has to consider factors that result in an abnormal response to fecal soiling to answer the question, “Who lets themselves be found in feces?” Those who let themselves be found in stool can be classified into 4 broad categories: those who are not aware that there is a problem, those who cannot move, those who will not move, and those who do not move.</p>
<p class="subheads-subhead-2">Not Aware</p>
<p class="body-text">Among the most obvious causes that contribute to an individual’s risk of being covered in feces is impairment of one’s ability to know that there is a problem. Impaired sensation, as can occur with cord paralysis or neurologic deficits (particularly if accompanied by anosmia), can limit awareness of the physical cues that signal the impending evacuation of fecal waste. Severe cognitive impairment or any cause of impaired consciousness (eg, encephalopathy, infection, metabolic disorder, and hypoxia) that interferes with an individual’s awareness of his or her environment is a clear risk factor for being found in feces.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref5">5</a></span> Intoxication, in particular, should be kept in mind as a cause of an altered, but reversible, sensorium that may have interfered with a person’s ability to properly control evacuation of solid waste and to clean up after a bowel movement. Organophosphate poisoning provides a good example of this problem, as the cholinergic surge associated with this toxidrome results in diffuse diarrhea and an altered sensorium that impairs one’s response to the bowel activity.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref9">9</a></span></p>
<p class="subheads-subhead-2">Cannot Move</p>
<p class="body-text">Some patients are aware that they have soiled themselves but are unable to clean up due to various physical impairments. These abnormalities are typically fairly obvious and usually result in the patient’s requiring assistance from others for numerous activities (including maintenance of proper toileting). Conditions that can interfere with an individual’s ability to maintain adequate personal hygiene include fractures, muscle weakness, strokes, morbid obesity, ataxia, poor coordination, hypoxia, and physical restrictions (eg, restraints, traction). In fact, fecal accidents are not uncommon in orthopedic wards where patients require significant assistance with toileting due to restricted mobility.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref10">10</a></span></p>
<p class="subheads-subhead-2">Will Not Move</p>
<p class="body-text"> Patients in this category are aware that they are covered in stool and are free from any physical limitation that might have impaired proper personal hygiene but still will not address the fact that they are covered in fecal matter. The causes underlying their inaction are usually difficult to detect, as they usually involve impairment of higher-order cognitive functions. Potential causes of abnormal evacuation behavior include personality disorders (that lead to problems with socialization and aggression and are linked with affective disorders and obsessive-compulsive disorder) and other types of psychopathology (eg, psychotic illnesses such as schizophrenia and catatonia) that affect the volitional component of defecation.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref6">6</a>,<a href="#ref11">11</a></span> For example, a hospitalized patient without physical limitations might purposefully defecate in bed as an expression of anger (usually directed at his or her nurse), or an individual might soil himself rather than use a public bathroom because of underlying social anxiety.</p>
<p class="subheads-subhead-2">Do Not Move</p>
<p class="body-text">Patients in the do-not-move category are similar to those that will not move in that they are aware and capable, but their reasons for their abnormal toileting behaviors and failure to avoid being covered in bodily excretions are more complex and multidimensional. The multifactorial nature of such behavioral disturbances results in more systemic dysfunction than in those with isolated psychopathology (eg, a combination of poverty and mild dementia in which people are forced to live in difficult situations with insufficient supplies and services). In this setting, lack of personal hygiene is a byproduct of global deficiencies, yet the individual remains aware of his or her condition. Evidence of social squalor and poor personal hygiene has been described in the medical literature and viewed as having multiple etiologies.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref12">12</a></span> In particular, the terms <span class="italic">Diogenes syndrome</span> or <span class="italic">senile breakdown</span> have been used to describe elderly individuals with gross self-neglect, apathy, and social isolation. Although many with this syndrome have normal intelligence and no evidence of psychiatric disorders or frontal impairment,<span class="superscript">13,14</span> such reclusive individuals who live in filth are known to social workers who often shepherd them toward contact with the medical community.</p>
<p class="subheads-subhead-1"><span class="bold">WHAT HAPPENED TO M</span><span class="forcelc">s</span><span class="bold"> A?</span></p>
<p class="body-text">Ms A’s dramatic presentation with manifestations of extreme self-neglect can serve as a stimulus for discussion of the evaluation and management of patients who are covered in feces.</p>
<p class="body-text">The initial approach involves bathing the patient to remove barriers to care that result from health care workers’ attitudes toward fecal contamination and addressing the patient’s general medical needs. It is particularly important to recognize that these individuals often present in crisis; they frequently require volume repletion and correction of electrolyte abnormalities.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref13">13</a>,<a href="#ref14">14</a></span> As many such patients present after a fall and have impaired mobility<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref14">14</a></span> and rhabdomyolysis, orthopedic evaluations need to be conducted. Careful nutritional support is imperative; this often requires repletion of caloric and vitamin deficiencies, being mindful of the risk of a refeeding syndrome.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref15">15</a></span></p>
<p class="body-text">Specific therapies are predicated on the cause of the condition. Patients should be screened for conditions that result in abnormal stool production and for abnormalities that impair response time. Treatment of conditions that affect response time may involve treatment of reversible causes of an altered sensorium for a patient in the not aware category. Assistance from orthopedics, physical therapy, neurology, or medical services may be required for those patients who cannot move. Treatment of patients who will not move or do not move despite having normal capabilities requires a multidisciplinary approach.</p>
<p class="body-text">Ms A required an open reduction and internal fixation to repair her hip fracture following a fall. Because of her heroic efforts to maintain hydration, her renal function was preserved, and she did not develop rhabdomyolysis (her creatine kinase level was 255 U/L). She did not manifest gastrointestinal abnormalities. Since she was able to travel between rooms despite her hip fracture, a lack of mobility was unlikely to be the primary reason that she was found covered in stool. Ms A’s social situation was more consistent with the do-not-move causes of being found in excrement rather than those of the cannot move category.</p>
<p class="body-text">Although Ms A did not exhibit syllogomania (excessive hoarding of rubbish) that is often seen in cases of Diogenes syndrome, her presentation was otherwise quite consistent with this diagnosis. Moreover, she came to medical attention following a fall (the most common cause for medical care in the original series of patients with this syndrome).<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref13">13</a></span> Despite taking great pains to maintain hydration, Ms A was unconcerned by the excrement and stench that surrounded her. She was of sufficient intelligence to mask her defects in executive function on an initial cursory evaluation; follow-up neuropsychiatric examinations uncovered her executive dysfunction. These findings, combined with defects in spatial cognition, language, mood, speech (dysarthria), and movement (ataxia), and results of imaging studies (that showed normal brain volume but cerebellar aplasia) pointed to the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome as the underlying cause of her presentation.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref16">16</a></span> Prior to the description of this syndrome in 1998,<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref16">16</a></span> little would have been made of her cerebellar aplasia except for the fact that it may have contributed to her dysmetria (despite subtle evidence from animal models that the cerebellum was involved in more than just motor function). While electrical stimulation of the vermis and fastigial nucleus can promote grooming behavior in rats and cats, mice with cerebellar degeneration exhibit impaired grooming behavior.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref17">17</a></span> In a sense, these studies foreshadowed Ms A’s presentation. Her treatment required a multidisciplinary approach to promote better grooming behaviors and to ensure her safety, while still maintaining her independence.</p>
<p class="subheads-subhead-1"><span class="bold">WHAT SHOULD THE NEUROPSYCHIATRIC WORKUP OF A PATIENT FOUND IN FECES INVOLVE?</span></p>
<p class="body-text">Determination of an appropriate neuropsychiatric workup begins with the localization of a patient’s specific cognitive and behavioral deficits, given that different neuropsychiatric disease processes may selectively affect specific brain systems. One difference between the classic localization strategy for detection of neurologic deficits by neurologists and the localization of deficits by cognitive and behavioral neurologists and neuropsychiatrists is that the latter try to identify neural systems and brain circuits.</p>
<p class="body-text">One commonly used test, the MMSE,<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref1">1</a>,<a href="#ref18">18</a></span> was originally designed as a screening tool for detection of dementia in the primary care setting. While it can be useful in uncovering moderate-to-severe impairments, it is insensitive to deficits in executive function<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref19">19</a></span> (eg, a patient with frontal lobe impairment may score 28/30 on the MMSE, and the test may not detect behavioral problems or an ability to return home safely). Thus, more extensive mental status testing is required, as was done subsequently in the case of Ms A.</p>
<p class="body-text">Ms A’s neuropsychiatric consultant noted that she was awake, alert, oriented, and cooperative and that she had normal naming, repetition, comprehension, and intact 3-word recall. These observations suggested that she did not have a global acute confusional state. However, she was unable to copy pentagons, a star, or a 3-dimensional cube; this indicated an impairment of visuospatial processing. Most notably, she was unable to perform serial <span class="italic">7</span>s, her reverse auditory digit span was markedly reduced in relation to her forward span (forward auditory span 7 and reverse auditory digit span 2), she demonstrated perseveration on Luria alternating motor sequences (fist-side-palm), and she had decreased verbal fluency (2 <span class="italic">F</span> words in 1 minute), as well as poor planning and concrete thinking on a clock-drawing task. Last, she displayed poor judgment. All of these latter impairments represented different aspects of executive dysfunction and localized her dysfunction to frontal networks (ie, the frontal lobes and their connections).</p>
<p class="body-text">Executive functions are a set of higher-order, “top-down” cerebral processes (ie, they act as the chief executive officer of the brain<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref20">20</a></span> that allows one to plan, prioritize, allocate, and manage competing cognitive resources [eg, attention, organization, initiation, planning, monitoring, set-shifting, and affect regulation]).<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref20">20</a></span></p>
<p class="body-text">The neural circuits that mediate executive function are widely distributed throughout the brain; as a result, they are extremely sensitive to the effects of systemic illness. The differential diagnosis of executive dysfunction is broad (<span class="callout"><a target="_blank" onclick="createFigure('t1'); return false;" href="#">Table 1</a></span>); it overlaps significantly with that of acute delirium, since there is a tight link between the systems that oversee the brain’s arousal and executive systems. Assessment also involves a thorough review of medications; detection of sedative-hypnotics and anticholinergics is crucial, as they are frequent iatrogenic instigators of executive dysfunction. Then, screening (for electrolyte imbalances, infection, inflammation, or endocrine dysfunction) is recommended, followed by neuroimaging and/or neurophysiologic studies, as clinically indicated. More invasive testing (eg, lumbar puncture, arterial blood gas) or tests for rarer causes of executive dysfunction (eg, paraneoplastic disease, neurodegenerative or neurogenetic disorders) should be considered on a case-by-case basis.</p>
<div id="figure" class="right">
<a target="_blank" onclick="createFigure('t1'); return false;" href="#"><img border="0" id="t1" alt="Table 1" src="12f01341t1.gif" /></a>
<p class="click-to-enlarge">Click figure to enlarge</p>
</div>
<p class="body-text">One might also consider screening for anosmia, although anosmia alone is unlikely to cause unawareness of fecal incontinence in the absence of impaired executive function. Increasingly, impairment of olfactory function has been detected in a number of neurodegenerative disorders (eg, Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease)<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref21">21–23</a></span>; furthermore, given the anatomic proximity of the olfactory bulb and the orbitofrontal cortex, many diseases and conditions (eg, traumatic brain injury, tumor) simultaneously present with olfactory impairment and executive dysfunction.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref22">22</a></span> Several standardized tests of olfactory function are currently available<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref22">22</a></span>; however, simple materials (such as coffee and toothpaste) are readily available for brief bedside evaluations. Last, it is also important to note that if olfactory dysfunction is identified, intranasal causes of anosmia should be considered.</p>
<p class="body-text">In the case of Ms A, basic screening for electrolyte imbalance, endocrine dysfunction, infection, and inflammation were unremarkable. Brain magnetic resonance imaging (<span class="callout"><a target="_blank" onclick="createFigure('f1'); return false;" href="#">Figure 1 A–C</a></span>) revealed remarkably little cortical atrophy (with possible mild biparietal atrophy) and no evidence of subcortical white matter disease to suggest a cerebrovascular cause of her executive dysfunction. Unexpectedly, however, Ms A was found to have complete absence of the cerebellum (ie, cerebellar agenesis).</p>
<div id="figure" class="right">
<a target="_blank" onclick="createFigure('f1'); return false;" href="#"><img border="0" id="f1" alt="Figure 1" src="12f01341f1.jpg" /></a>
<p class="click-to-enlarge">Click figure to enlarge</p>
</div>
<p class="body-text">For decades, this rare congenital condition was believed to be asymptomatic. However, cerebellar agenesis has subsequently been shown to be associated with a range of motor, cognitive, and behavioral abnormalities (including developmental motor delay, mild ataxia, eye movement abnormalities, language impairment, “autistic-like” behaviors, and, notably, both visuospatial and executive dysfunction [perseveration, poor abstract reasoning, impaired working memory, reduced verbal fluency]), as was seen in the case of Ms A.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref24">24</a></span> These abnormalities have in fact been identified in a wide range of developmental, acquired, and degenerative cerebellar conditions; taken together, they form a clinical entity known as the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref24">24</a></span> Initially described by Schmahmann and Sherman<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref16">16</a></span> in 1998, the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome is characterized by 4 major cognitive and behavioral features: executive dysfunction, visuospatial disorganization, language dysfunction (typically agrammatism, mild anomia, and dysprosodia), and “affective overshoot,” consisting either of emotional blunting or behavioral disinhibition. Deficits in each of these domains are thought to arise from dysfunction of different corticocerebellar regulatory networks, in which the cerebellum serves to regulate and refine cognitive and affective processes (described by Schmahmann as the “universal cerebellar transform”<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref24">24</a>[p374])</span> in a manner similar to its traditionally described role in regulation of movement. Thus, as Alexander and colleagues<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref26">26</a></span> described parallel motor, cognitive, and affective circuits in their model of the basal ganglia (which now serves as a basis for understanding neuropsychiatric disorders such as Tourette’s disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder),<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref26">26</a>,<a href="#ref27">27</a></span> it appears that the cerebellum has similar nonmotor connections. Therefore, one should consider deficits in frontal, parietal, and limbic networks as potentially arising from dysfunction of corticocerebellar circuits. Early lesion studies, followed by recent functional neuroimaging studies, have proposed that each of these circuits is localized in different cerebellar regions, with lateral cerebellar hemispheres mediating the different cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome components and medial structures (cerebellar vermis) mediating affective and autonomic circuits.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref28">28</a></span></p>
<p class="body-text">In summary, the identification of cerebellar agenesis during the neuropsychiatric workup is not merely a “radiologic incidentaloma,” but instead it is likely to be a key contributor to Ms A’s presentation (both in terms of her unsteady gait that may have precipitated her fall, as well as her persistent cognitive dysfunction).</p>
<p class="body-text">One should remain concerned by the possibility of a superimposed early degenerative process that could compound Ms A’s baseline executive dysfunction and explain the “why now” question of her case. Thus, continued cognitive evaluations to monitor for subsequent cognitive decline would be recommended.</p>
<p class="subheads-subhead-1"><span class="bold">WHAT ARE THE SOCIAL IMPLICATIONS OF BEING FOUND IN FECES?</span></p>
<p class="body-text">The dual practices of carefully applied mindfulness and attention to personal insights are especially important for all health care providers when faced with patient presentations that threaten and disrupt established and accepted cultural standards. Certainly this is true for the patient who is covered in his/her own feces. Such presentations commonly evoke feelings of disgust and near universal repulsion. In fact, a content search of these topics using medical search engines consistently yields articles with the key terms of <span class="italic">disgust</span>, <span class="italic">repulsion</span>, and <span class="italic">grotesque</span>.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref29">29–31</a></span> This finding suggests that providers who care for any patient who is covered in feces ought to steel themselves for what may be seemingly universal responses. It serves no one, neither patient nor colleague, to act as if these responses are not present. As the famous poet and pediatrician William Carlos Williams stressed, it is far better to know and to grapple with what we feel when treating our patients than to act as if these feelings are forbidden or not present at all.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref32">32</a></span></p>
<p class="body-text">Evolutionary theorists have suggested that there is teleological repulsion triggered by the stench of human and other mammalian excrement that derives from primitive brain regions; this information is then efficiently and powerfully coded within our neurobiological architecture as something to be avoided.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref33">33</a></span> As excrement can be associated with disease and is indeed implicated as a vector for many infectious processes, this primitive repulsion makes evolutionary sense.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref31">31</a>,<a href="#ref33">33</a>,<a href="#ref34">34</a></span> Therefore, medical professionals should remain vigilant so as not to allow a seemingly hard-wired response to interfere with patient care.</p>
<p class="body-text">Additional concerns include the theorized and even measured economic devaluing that occurs with the sensation of disgust or repulsion. Economists, such as Alvin Roth,<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref35">35</a></span> have noted that modern culture tends to place less value on ideas or practices that are universally off-putting. Other theorists<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref36">36</a></span> have noted particular aspects of our culture that are especially likely to be devalued on the basis of these principles, and fecal matter is often mentioned as a “core disgust elicitor.” This view suggests that physicians and indeed entire systems of care may respond with an overall sense that such patients who present like Ms A are somehow worth less.</p>
<p class="body-text">It also seems clear from the literature that nurses may be more comfortable with these issues than are physicians. The majority of articles addressing how best to understand and care for these patients are found in the nursing literature.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref29">29–31</a></span> One might postulate that doctors view these issues in more emotionally sterile ways than do nurses, as a defense against the strong feelings that are evoked.</p>
<p class="body-text">Finally, literature also suggests that despite the seemingly primitive response to feces described above, neuroimaging investigations show that reactions to feces among human subjects involve activation of the prefrontal cortex in a manner similar to the contemplation of morally charged topics, such as incest.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref37">37</a></span> This reaction is different from the more neurobiologically primitive response to fears of infection that has also been studied.<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref37">37</a></span> Thus, one could argue that the apparent sterility with which the health care system might treat such patients stems from a moral judgment that is made regarding the hygiene of the patient. As moral judgments clearly and often negatively affect care, it is important that doctors be wary of these judgments when facing such patient presentations<span class="htm-cite"><a href="#ref38">38</a></span> and that they investigate the underpinnings for being found in feces to guide their workup and treatment.</p>
<p class="references-references-head"><span class="bold">REFERENCES</span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-1-9"><a name="ref1"></a>1. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. <span class="italic">J Psychiatr Res</span>. 1975;12(3):189–198.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=1202204&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6">doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-1-9"><a name="ref2"></a>2. Thielman NM, Guerrant RL. Clinical practice: acute infectious diarrhea. <span class="italic">N Engl J Med</span>. 2004;350(1):38–47.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=14702426&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMcp031534">doi:10.1056/NEJMcp031534</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-1-9"><a name="ref3"></a>3. Schiller LR. Diarrhea and malabsorption in the elderly. <span class="italic">Gastroenterol Clin North Am</span>. 2009;38(3):481–502.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=19699409&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gtc.2009.06.008">doi:10.1016/j.gtc.2009.06.008</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-1-9"><a name="ref4"></a>4. Tomassetti P, Migliori M, Lalli S, et al. Epidemiology, clinical features and diagnosis of gastroenteropancreatic endocrine tumours. <span class="italic">Ann Oncol</span>. 2001;12(suppl 2):S95–S99.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=11762360&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/annonc/12.suppl_2.S95">doi:10.1093/annonc/12.suppl_2.S95</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-1-9"><a name="ref5"></a>5. Wald A. Clinical practice. Fecal incontinence in adults. <span class="italic">N Engl J Med</span>. 2007;356(16):1648–1655.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=17442907&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMcp067041">doi:10.1056/NEJMcp067041</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-1-9"><a name="ref6"></a>6. Matson JL, LoVullo SV. Encopresis, soiling and constipation in children and adults with developmental disability. <span class="italic">Res Dev Disabil</span>. 2009;30(4):799–807.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=19162439&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2008.12.001">doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2008.12.001</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-1-9"><a name="ref7"></a>7. Lembo A, Camilleri M. Chronic constipation. <span class="italic">N Engl J Med</span>. 2003;349(14):1360–1368.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=14523145&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMra020995">doi:10.1056/NEJMra020995</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-1-9"><a name="ref8"></a>8. Rao SS, Go JT. Update on the management of constipation in the elderly: new treatment options. <span class="italic">Clin Interv Aging</span>. 2010;5:163–171.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=20711435&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.2147/CIA.S8100">doi:10.2147/CIA.S8100</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-1-9"><a name="ref9"></a>9. Karalliedde L, Baker D, Marrs TC. Organophosphate-induced intermediate syndrome: aetiology and relationships with myopathy. <span class="italic">Toxicol Rev</span>. 2006;25(1):1–14.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=16856766&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.2165/00139709-200625010-00001">doi:10.2165/00139709-200625010-00001</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref10"></a>10. Hossain MA, Pearce R, Bircher MD. Perineal hygiene in patients with pelvic fractures. <span class="italic">Injury</span>. 2008;39(8):889–892.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=18054015&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.injury.2007.07.027">doi:10.1016/j.injury.2007.07.027</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref11"></a>11. Edwards NI, Jones D. The prevalence of faecal incontinence in older people living at home. <span class="italic">Age Ageing</span>. 2001;30(6):503–507.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=11742780&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ageing/30.6.503">doi:10.1093/ageing/30.6.503</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref12"></a>12. Clark J. Senile squalor syndrome: two unusual cases. <span class="italic">J R Soc Med</span>. 1999;92(3):138–140. <span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=10396262&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref13"></a>13. Clark AN, Mankikar GD, Gray I. Diogenes syndrome: a clinical study of gross neglect in old age. <span class="italic">Lancet</span>. 1975;1(7903):366–368.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=46514&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(75)91280-5">doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(75)91280-5</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref14"></a>14. Cooney C, Hamid W. Review: Diogenes syndrome. <span class="italic">Age Ageing</span>. 1995;24(5):451–453.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=8669353&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ageing/24.5.451">doi:10.1093/ageing/24.5.451</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref15"></a>15. Boateng AA, Sriram K, Meguid MM, et al. Refeeding syndrome: treatment considerations based on collective analysis of literature case reports. <span class="italic">Nutrition</span>. 2010;26(2):156–167.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=20122539&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nut.2009.11.017">doi:10.1016/j.nut.2009.11.017</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref16"></a>16. Schmahmann JD, Sherman JC. The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. <span class="italic">Brain</span>. 1998;121(Pt 4):561–579.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=9577385&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/121.4.561">doi:10.1093/brain/121.4.561</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref17"></a>17. Strazielle C, Lalonde R. Grooming in Lurcher mutant mice. <span class="italic">Physiol Behav</span>. 1998;64(1):57–61.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=9661982&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00014-6">doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00014-6</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref18"></a>18. Crum RM, Anthony JC, Bassett SS, et al. Population-based norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educational level. <span class="italic">JAMA</span>. 1993;269(18):2386–2391.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=8479064&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.1993.03500180078038">doi:10.1001/jama.1993.03500180078038</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref19"></a>19. Tombaugh TN, McIntyre NJ. The Mini-Mental State Examination: a comprehensive review. <span class="italic">J Am Geriatr Soc</span>. 1992;40(9):922–935. <span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=1512391&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref20"></a>20. Daffner KR, Searl MM. The dysexecutive syndromes. <span class="italic">Handb Clin Neurol</span>. 2008;88:249–267.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=18631695&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0072-9752(07)88012-2">doi:10.1016/S0072-9752(07)88012-2</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref21"></a>21. Hawkes C. Olfaction in neurodegenerative disorder. <span class="italic">Adv Otorhinolaryngol</span>. 2006;63:133–151. <span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=16733338&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref22"></a>22. Duffy LS, Stern TA. The role of olfaction in medicine and psychiatry. <span class="italic">Med Psychiatry</span>. 1999;2:15–22.</p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref23"></a>23. Finelli PF, Mair RG. Disturbances of smell and taste. In: Bradley WG, Daroff RB, Fenichel GM, et al, eds. <span class="italic">Bradley: Neurology in Clinical Practice</span>. vol 1. 5th ed. Philadelphia, PA: Butterworth Heinemann Elsevier; 2008:255–262.</p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref24"></a>24. Schmahmann JD. Disorders of the cerebellum: ataxia, dysmetria of thought, and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. <span class="italic">J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci</span>. 2004;16(3):367–378.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=15377747&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16.3.367">doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16.3.367</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref25"></a>25. Schmahmann JD, Weilburg JB, Sherman JC. The neuropsychiatry of the cerebellum: insights from the clinic. <span class="italic">Cerebellum</span>. 2007;6(3):254–267.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=17786822&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14734220701490995">doi:10.1080/14734220701490995</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref26"></a>26. Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. <span class="italic">Annu Rev Neurosci</span>. 1986;9(1):357–381.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=3085570&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041">doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref27"></a>27. Mink JW. Neurobiology of basal ganglia and Tourette syndrome: basal ganglia circuits and thalamocortical outputs. <span class="italic">Adv Neurol</span>. 2006;99:89–98. <span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=16536354&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref28"></a>28. Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD. Evidence for topographic organization in the cerebellum of motor control versus cognitive and affective processing. <span class="italic">Cortex</span>. 2010;46(7):831–844.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=20152963&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.008">doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.008</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref29"></a>29. Holmes D, Perron A, O’Byrne P. Understanding disgust in nursing: abjection, self, and the other. <span class="italic">Res Theory Nurs Pract</span>. 2006;20(4):305–315.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=17190117&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/rtnp-v20i4a006">doi:10.1891/rtnp-v20i4a006</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref30"></a>30. Norton C. Nurses, bowel continence, stigma, and taboos. <span class="italic">J Wound Ostomy Continence Nurs</span>. 2004;31(2):85–94. <span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=15209431&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref31"></a>31. van Dongen E. It isn’t something to yodel about, but it exists! Faeces, nurses, social relations and status within a mental hospital. <span class="italic">Aging Ment Health</span>. 2001;5(3):205–215.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=11575059&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607860120064952">doi:10.1080/13607860120064952</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref32"></a>32. Williams WC. <span class="italic">The Doctor Stories</span>. Coles R, ed. New York, NY: New Directions Books; 1984.</p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref33"></a>33. Rozin P, Fallon AE. A perspective on disgust. <span class="italic">Psychol Rev</span>. 1987;94(1):23–41.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=3823304&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.23">doi:10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.23</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref34"></a>34. Prokop P, Fancovicova J. The effect of owning animals on perceived vulnerability to, and avoidance of, parasitic diseases in humans. <span class="italic">J Individ Differ</span>. 2011;32(3):129–136. <span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000045">doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000045</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref35"></a>35. Roth A. Repugnance as a constraint on markets. <span class="italic">J Econ Perspect</span>. 2007;21(3):37–58. <span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/jep.21.3.37">doi:10.1257/jep.21.3.37</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref36"></a>36. Tybur JM, Lieberman D, Griskevicius V. Microbes, mating, and morality: individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. <span class="italic">J Pers Soc Psychol</span>. 2009;97(1):103–122.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=19586243&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015474">doi:10.1037/a0015474</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref37"></a>37. Schaich Borg J, Lieberman D, Kiehl KA. Infection, incest, and iniquity: investigating the neural correlates of disgust and morality. <span class="italic">J Cogn Neurosci</span>. 2008;20(9):1529–1546.<span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=18345982&dopt=Abstract">PubMed</a> <a href="
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20109">doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20109</a></span></p>
<p class="references-references-text-10-99"><a name="ref3"></a>38. Hill TE. How clinicians make (or avoid) moral judgments of patients: implications of the evidence for relationships and research. <span class="italic">Philos Ethics Humanit Med</span>. 2010;5:11. <span class="pubmed-crossref"><a href="
http://www.peh-med.com/content/5/1/11">doi:10.1186/1747-5341-5-11</a> <a href="
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20618947">PubMed</a></span></p>
</div>
</div>